Display options
Share it on

Front Psychol. 2014 Dec 04;5:1395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01395. eCollection 2014.

Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?.

Frontiers in psychology

Sarah R Beck, Nicola Cutting, Ian A Apperly, Zoe Demery, Leila Iliffe, Sonia Rishi, Jackie Chappell

Affiliations

  1. School of Psychology, University of Birmingham Birmingham, UK.
  2. School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham Birmingham, UK.

PMID: 25538650 PMCID: PMC4255490 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01395

Abstract

In three studies, we explored the retention and transfer of tool-making knowledge, learnt from an adult demonstration, to other temporal and task contexts. All studies used a variation of a task in which children had to make a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tall transparent tube. Children who failed to innovate the hook tool independently saw a demonstration. In Study 1, we tested children aged 4-6 years (N = 53) who had seen the original demonstration 3 months earlier. Performance was excellent at the second time, indicating that children's knowledge was retained over the 3 month period. In Studies 2 and 3 we explored transfer of the new knowledge to other tasks. In Study 2, children were given two variants of the apparatus that differed in surface characteristics (e.g., shape and color). Participants generalized their knowledge to these new apparatuses even though the new pipecleaner also differed in size and color. Five- to 6-year-olds (N = 22) almost always transferred their knowledge to problems where the same tool had to be made. Younger, 3- to 5-year-olds' (N = 46), performance was more variable. In Study 3, 4- to 7-year-olds (N = 146) saw a demonstration of hook making with a pipecleaner, but then had to make a tool by combining pieces of wooden dowel (or vice versa: original training on dowel, transfer to pipecleaner). Children did not transfer their tool-making knowledge to the new material. Children retained tool-making knowledge over time and transferred their knowledge to new situations in which they needed to make a similar tool from similar materials, but not different materials. We concluded that children's ability to use tool-making knowledge in novel situations is likely to depend on memory and analogical reasoning, with the latter continuing to develop during middle childhood.

Keywords: analogy; cognitive development; innovation; pedagogy; problem solving; social learning; tool use

References

  1. Cognition. 2011 May;119(2):301-6 - PubMed
  2. Dev Psychol. 2000 Jul;36(4):473-84 - PubMed
  3. Dev Psychol. 2011 May;47(3):834-40 - PubMed
  4. Folia Primatol (Basel). 1990;54(1-2):86-99 - PubMed
  5. J Exp Child Psychol. 2010 May;106(1):82-97 - PubMed
  6. Infant Behav Dev. 2013 Feb;36(1):25-31 - PubMed
  7. Proc Biol Sci. 2004 Feb 7;271 Suppl 3:S88-90 - PubMed
  8. Infant Behav Dev. 2012 Apr;35(2):195-204 - PubMed
  9. J Exp Child Psychol. 2014 Oct;126:384-94 - PubMed
  10. J Exp Child Psychol. 2014 Sep;125:110-7 - PubMed
  11. Infant Behav Dev. 2006 Jan;29(1):136-9 - PubMed
  12. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011 Aug;109(4):497-511 - PubMed
  13. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013 Oct 07;368(1630):20120409 - PubMed
  14. Science. 2002 Aug 9;297(5583):981 - PubMed
  15. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2000;65(4):i-vi, 1-204 - PubMed
  16. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Apr;13(4):148-53 - PubMed
  17. Psychol Sci. 2011 Feb;22(2):267-73 - PubMed
  18. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Jun 23;106(25):10370-5 - PubMed
  19. Child Dev. 2003 Jan-Feb;74(1):48-62 - PubMed

Publication Types