Display options
Share it on

Psychol Res. 2021 Nov;85(8):3040-3047. doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01453-7. Epub 2021 Jan 03.

Does attention solve the "apples-and-oranges" problems of judging task difficulty and task order?.

Psychological research

Cory A Potts, David A Rosenbaum

Affiliations

  1. Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802, USA. [email protected].
  2. Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside, CA, 95251, USA.

PMID: 33389043 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-020-01453-7

Abstract

How do we compare the difficulty of different kinds of tasks, and how we do sequence tasks of different kinds when the basis for the ordering is the tasks' difficulty levels? The ability to do these things requires a common currency, but the identity of that currency, if it exists, is unknown. We hypothesized that people may believe that the time that attention is paid to tasks enables people to compare and sequence tasks of different kinds. To evaluate this hypothesis, we tested three groups of participants. One group estimated the proportion of time that performance of a task requires attention-what we called attention time proportions or ATPs. We obtained ATPs for tasks that were "more intellectual" (counting) and others that were "more physical" (locomotion). Two additional groups made 2-alternative-forced-choice decisions about the relative ease and preferred sequencing of all possible pairs of tasks for which ATPs were independently obtained. We found that ATPs predicted judgments of task difficulty and preferred task order.

© 2021. Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature.

References

  1. Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. - PubMed
  2. Beaty, R., Frieler, K., Norgaard, M., Merseal, H., MacDonald, M., & Weiss, D. (2020). Spontaneous melodic productions of expert musicians contain sequencing biases seen in language production. psyarxiv.com. - PubMed
  3. Behaghel, O. (1909). Beziehungen zwischen umfang und reihenfolge von satzgliedern. Indogermanische Forschungen, 25, 110. - PubMed
  4. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624. - PubMed
  5. Chen, H., & Wyble, B. (2016). Attribute amnesia reflects a lack of memory consolidation for attended information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 225. - PubMed
  6. Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition, 78, 94–98. - PubMed
  7. Dunn, T. L., Lutes, D. J., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Metacognitive evaluation in the avoidance of demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1372. - PubMed
  8. Feghhi, I., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2019). Judging the subjective difficulty of different kinds of tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 983–994. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000653 . - PubMed
  9. Feghhi, I., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2020). Effort-avoidance isn’t simply error-avoidance. Psychological Research PsychologischeForschung. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01331-2 (Epub ahead of print). - PubMed
  10. Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont: Brooks Cole. - PubMed
  11. Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 303–327. - PubMed
  12. Gray, W. D., Sims, C. R., Fu, W. T., & Schoelles, M. J. (2006). The soft constraints hypothesis: A rational analysis approach to resource allocation for interactive behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 461. - PubMed
  13. Grondin, S. (Ed.). (2008). Psychology of time. Emerald: Bingley. - PubMed
  14. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. - PubMed
  15. Hopstaken, J. F., van der Linden, D., Bakker, A. B., Kompier, M. A., & Leung, Y. K. (2016). Shifts in attention during mental fatigue: Evidence from subjective, behavioral, physiological, and eye-tracking data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 878–889. - PubMed
  16. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. - PubMed
  17. Knowles, J. R. (1980). Enzyme-catalyzed phosphoryl transfer reactions. Annual Review of Biochemistry, 49, 877–919. - PubMed
  18. Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665. - PubMed
  19. Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–726. - PubMed
  20. Lurquin, J. H., & Miyake, A. (2017). Challenges to ego-depletion research go beyond the replication crisis: A need for tackling the conceptual crisis. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 568. - PubMed
  21. MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 226. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226 . - PubMed
  22. Metcalfe, J., & Greene, M. J. (2007). Metacognition of agency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 184. - PubMed
  23. Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326. - PubMed
  24. Morsella, E., Wilson, L. E., Berger, C. C., Honhongva, M., Gazzaley, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). Subjective aspects of cognitive control at different stages of processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1807–1824. - PubMed
  25. Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198. - PubMed
  26. Potts, C. A., Pastel, S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018). How are cognitive and physical difficulty compared? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 500–511. - PubMed
  27. Rosenbaum, D. A., & Bui, B. (2019). Does task sustainability provide a unified measure of subjective task difficulty? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 1980–1987. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01631-8 . - PubMed
  28. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological review, 84(2), 127. - PubMed
  29. Sidarus, N., & Haggard, P. (2016). Difficult action decisions reduce the sense of agency: A study using the Eriksen flanker task. Acta Psychologica, 166, 1–11. - PubMed
  30. Spelke, E., Hirst, W., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–230. - PubMed
  31. Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 379–384. - PubMed
  32. Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., & Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50, 433–441. - PubMed
  33. Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1996). The role of attention in time estimation processes. In Advances in Psychology (vol. 115, pp. 143–164). North-Holland. - PubMed

MeSH terms

Publication Types