Display options
Share it on

Br J Psychol. 2022 Feb;113(1):25-48. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12522. Epub 2021 Jul 14.

The entertainment value of conspiracy theories.

British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)

Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Joline Ligthart, Sabine Rosema, Yang Xu

Affiliations

  1. VU Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
  2. The Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement (NSCR), Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

PMID: 34260744 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12522

Abstract

Many citizens around the globe believe conspiracy theories. Why are conspiracy theories so appealing? Here, we propose that conspiracy theories elicit intense emotions independent of emotional valence. People therefore find conspiracy theories entertaining - that is, narratives that people perceive as interesting, exciting, and attention-grabbing - and such entertainment appraisals are positively associated with belief in them. Five studies supported these ideas. Participants were exposed to either a conspiratorial or a non-conspiratorial text about the Notre Dame fire (Study 1) or the death of Jeffrey Epstein (preregistered Study 2). The conspiratorial text elicited stronger entertainment appraisals and intense emotions (independent of emotional valence) than the non-conspiratorial text; moreover, entertainment appraisals mediated the effects of the manipulation on conspiracy beliefs. Study 3 indicated that participants endorsed stronger conspiracy beliefs when an election event was described in an entertaining rather than a boring manner. Subsequent findings revealed that both organisational (Study 4) and societal conspiracy beliefs (Study 5) are positively associated with sensation seeking - a trait characterised by a preference for exciting and intense experiences. We conclude that one reason why people believe conspiracy theories is because they find them entertaining.

© 2021 The Authors. British Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Psychological Society.

Keywords: conspiracy theories; emotional intensity; emotions; entertainment; sensation seeking

References

  1. Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637-647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160 - PubMed
  2. Bale, J. M. (2007). Political paranoia v. political realism: On distinguishing between bogus conspiracy theories and genuine conspiratorial politics. Patterns of Prejudice, 41, 45-60. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313220601118751 - PubMed
  3. Brashier, N. M., & Marsh, E. J. (2020). Judging truth. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 499-515. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050807 - PubMed
  4. Brotherton, R., & Eser, S. (2015). Bored to fears: Boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 1-5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.011 - PubMed
  5. Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic belief in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225. - PubMed
  6. Butter, M., & Knight, P. (2020). Routledge handbook of conspiracy theories. Oxon, UK: Routledge. - PubMed
  7. Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., & Golec de Zavala, A. (2016). Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, self-esteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 157-166. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615616170 - PubMed
  8. Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories: The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1289-1293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027 - PubMed
  9. Dechêne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2010). The truth about the truth: A meta-analytic review of the truth effect. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 238-257. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309352251 - PubMed
  10. Douglas, K. M., & Leite, A. C. (2017). Suspicion in the workplace: Organizational conspiracy theories and work-related outcomes. British Journal of Psychology, 108, 486-506. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12212 - PubMed
  11. Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 538-542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417718261 - PubMed
  12. Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in negative affect and well-being: The case for emotional intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 427-434. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.427 - PubMed
  13. Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 733-744. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630 - PubMed
  14. Golec de Zavala, A., & Cichocka, A. (2012). Collective narcissism and anti-semitism in Poland. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 15, 213-229. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430211420891 - PubMed
  15. Golec de Zavala, A., & Federico, C. M. (2018). Collective narcissism and the growth of conspiracy thinking over the course of the 2016 United States presidential election: A longitudinal analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 1011-1018. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2496 - PubMed
  16. Grebe, E., & Nattrass, N. (2012). AIDS conspiracy beliefs and unsafe sex in Cape Town. AIDS and Behavior, 16, 761-773. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-011-9958-2 - PubMed
  17. Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2013). The effect of high-anxiety situations on conspiracy thinking. Current Psychology, 32, 100-118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-013-9165-6 - PubMed
  18. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press. - PubMed
  19. Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Pugzles Lorch, E., & Donohew, R. L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure on sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 401-414. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0 - PubMed
  20. Hwang, Y., & Southwell, B. G. (2007). Can a personality trait predict talk about science? Science Communication, 29, 198-216. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547007308599 - PubMed
  21. Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalized political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28, 25-43. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1930 - PubMed
  22. Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2017). Too special to be duped: Need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 724-734. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2265 - PubMed
  23. Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2020). A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11, 1110-1118. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620934692 - PubMed
  24. Joireman, J. A., Fick, C. S., & Anderson, J. W. (2002). Sensation seeking and involvement in chess. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 509-515. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00052-6 - PubMed
  25. Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. (2014a). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9, e89177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 - PubMed
  26. Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprints. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018 - PubMed
  27. Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., Leite, A. C., & Schrader, T. (2019). Belief in conspiracy theories and intentions to engage in everyday crime. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58, 534-549. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12311 - PubMed
  28. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow, New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. - PubMed
  29. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263-291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185 - PubMed
  30. Kofta, M., Soral, W., & Bilewicz, M. (2020). What breeds conspiracy antisemitism? The role of political uncontrollability and uncertainty in the belief in Jewish conspiracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118, 900-918. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000183 - PubMed
  31. Kumar, V. K., Pekala, R. J., & Cummings, J. (1993). Sensation seeking, drug use and reported paranormal beliefs and experiences. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 685-691. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(93)90116-K - PubMed
  32. Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). “I know things they don’t know!”: The role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Social Psychology, 48, 160-173. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000306 - PubMed
  33. Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 1-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(87)90023-7 - PubMed
  34. Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617-625. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042 - PubMed
  35. Morris, R., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). The relationship between gambling, affinity, impulsivity, sensation seeking, superstition, and irrational beliefs: An empirical study among committed gamblers. Aloma, 31, 109-121. - PubMed
  36. Newheiser, A.-K., Farias, M., & Tausch, N. (2011). The functional nature of conspiracy beliefs: Examining the underpinnings of belief in the Da Vinci Code conspiracy. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 1007-1011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.011 - PubMed
  37. Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. (2014). Medical conspiracy theories and health behaviors in the United States. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 817-818. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.190 - PubMed
  38. Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 257-301. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018301 - PubMed
  39. Raihani, N. J., & Bell, V. (2018). An evolutionary perspective on paranoia. Nature Human Behavior, 3, 114-121. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0495-0 - PubMed
  40. Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 338-342. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1999.0386 - PubMed
  41. Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 256-279. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00067-9 - PubMed
  42. Smith, C. L., Johnson, J. L., & Hathaway, W. (2009). Personality contributions to belief in paranormal phenomena. Individual Differences Research, 7, 85-96. - PubMed
  43. Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845-851. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845 - PubMed
  44. Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy theories: Causes and cures. The Journal of Political Philosophy, 17, 202-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x - PubMed
  45. Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443-463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x - PubMed
  46. Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133, 572-585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006 - PubMed
  47. Van Boven, L., Kane, J., McGraw, A. P., & Dale, J. (2010). Feeling close: Emotional intensity reduces perceived psychological distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 872-885. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019262 - PubMed
  48. Van den Bos, K. (2009). Making sense of life: The existential self trying to deal with personal uncertainty. Psychological Inquiry, 20, 197-217. https://doi.org/10.1080/10478400903333411 - PubMed
  49. Van der Linden, S. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 171-173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045 - PubMed
  50. Van Harreveld, F., Rutjens, B., Schneider, I. K., Nohlen, H. U., & Keskinis, K. (2014). In doubt and disorderly: Ambivalence promotes compensatory perceptions of order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1666-1676. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036099 - PubMed
  51. Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2016). Sometimes inclusion breeds suspicion: Self-uncertainty and belongingness predict belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 267-279. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2157 - PubMed
  52. Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2018). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Oxon, UK: Routledge. - PubMed
  53. Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2020). An existential threat model of conspiracy theories. European Psychologist, 25, 16-25. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000381 - PubMed
  54. Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 753-761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3161 - PubMed
  55. Van Prooijen, J.-W., & De Vries, R. E. (2016). Organizational conspiracy beliefs: Implications for leadership styles and employee outcomes. Journal of Business and Psychology, 31, 479-491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-015-9428-3 - PubMed
  56. Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Memory Studies, 10, 323-333. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698017701615 - PubMed
  57. Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Douglas, K. M. (2018). Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 897-908. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2530 - PubMed
  58. Van Prooijen, J.-W., Douglas, K., & De Inocencio, C. (2018). Connecting the dots: Illusory pattern perception predicts beliefs in conspiracies and the supernatural. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 320-335. - PubMed
  59. Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Van Dijk, E. (2014). When consequence size predicts belief in conspiracy theories: The moderating role of perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 63-73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.006 - PubMed
  60. Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Van Vugt, M. (2018). Conspiracy theories: Evolved functions and psychological mechanisms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 770-788. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618774270 - PubMed
  61. Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322, 115-117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845 - PubMed
  62. Whitson, J. A., Galinsky, A. D., & Kay, A. (2015). The emotional roots of conspiratorial perceptions, system justification, and belief in the paranormal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 89-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.09.002 - PubMed
  63. Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459-482. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.920180503 - PubMed
  64. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. - PubMed
  65. Zuckerman, M., Eysenck, S., & Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Sensation seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 139-149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.46.1.139 - PubMed

MeSH terms

Publication Types